Thinking, Fast and Slow is a 2011 popular science book by psychologist Daniel Kahneman.
The book's main thesis is a differentiation between two modes of thought: "System 1" is fast, instinctive and emotional; "System 2" is slower, more deliberative, and more logical.
The book delineates rational and non-rational motivations or triggers associated with each type of thinking process, and how they complement each other, starting with Kahneman's own research on loss aversion. From framing choices to people's tendency to replace a difficult question with one which is easy to answer, the book summarizes several decades of research to suggest that people have too much confidence in human judgment.[1] Kahneman performed his own research, often in collaboration with Amos Tversky, which enriched his experience to write the book.[2][3] It covers different phases of his career: his early work concerning cognitive biases, his work on prospect theory and happiness, and with the Israel Defense Forces.
In the book's first section, Kahneman describes two different ways the brain forms thoughts:
System 1: Fast, automatic, frequent, emotional, stereotypic, unconscious. Examples (in order of complexity) of things system 1 can do:
determine that an object is at a greater distance than another
localize the source of a specific sound
complete the phrase "war and ..."
display disgust when seeing a gruesome image
solve 2+2=?
read text on a billboard
drive a car on an empty road
think of a good chess move (if you're a chess master)
understand simple sentences
System 2: Slow, effortful, infrequent, logical, calculating, conscious. Examples of things system 2 can do:
prepare yourself for the start of a sprint
direct your attention towards the clowns at the circus
direct your attention towards someone at a loud party
look for the woman with the grey hair
try to recognize a sound
sustain a faster-than-normal walking rate
determine the appropriateness of a particular behavior in a social setting
count the number of A's in a certain text
give someone your telephone number
park into a tight parking space
determine the price/quality ratio of two washing machines
determine the validity of a complex logical reasoning
solve 17 × 24
Kahneman describes a number of experiments which purport to examine the differences between these two thought systems and how they arrive at different results even given the same inputs. Terms and concepts include coherence, attention, laziness, association, jumping to conclusions, WYSIATI (What you see is all there is), and how one forms judgments. The System 1 vs. System 2 debate includes the reasoning or lack thereof for human decision making, with big implications for many areas including law and market research.[7]
The second section offers explanations for why humans struggle to think statistically. It begins by documenting a variety of situations in which we either arrive at binary decisions or fail to associate precisely reasonable probabilities with outcomes. Kahneman explains this phenomenon using the theory of heuristics. Kahneman and Tversky originally discussed this topic in their 1974 article titled Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases.[8]
Kahneman uses heuristics to assert that System 1 thinking involves associating new information with existing patterns, or thoughts, rather than creating new patterns for each new experience. For example, a child who has only seen shapes with straight edges might perceive an octagon when first viewing a circle. As a legal metaphor, a judge limited to heuristic thinking would only be able to think of similar historical cases when presented with a new dispute, rather than considering the unique aspects of that case. In addition to offering an explanation for the statistical problem, the theory also offers an explanation for human biases.
The "anchoring effect" names a tendency to be influenced by irrelevant numbers. Shown greater/lesser numbers, experimental subjects gave greater/lesser responses.[2] As an example, most people, when asked whether Gandhi was more than 114 years old when he died, will provide a much greater estimate of his age at death than others who were asked whether Gandhi was more or less than 35 years old. Experiments show that people's behavior is influenced, much more than they are aware, by irrelevant information.
The availability heuristic is a mental shortcut that occurs when people make judgments about the probability of events on the basis of how easy it is to think of examples. The availability heuristic operates on the notion that, "if you can think of it, it must be important". The availability of consequences associated with an action is related positively to perceptions of the magnitude of the consequences of that action. In other words, the easier it is to recall the consequences of something, the greater we perceive these consequences to be. Sometimes, this heuristic is beneficial, but the frequencies at which events come to mind are usually not accurate representations of the probabilities of such events in real life.[9][10]
Conjunction fallacy
System 1 is prone to substituting a simpler question for a difficult one. In what Kahneman terms their "best-known and most controversial" experiment, "the Linda problem," subjects were told about an imaginary Linda, young, single, outspoken, and intelligent, who, as a student, was very concerned with discrimination and social justice. They asked whether it was more probable that Linda is a bank teller or that she is a bank teller and an active feminist. The overwhelming response was that "feminist bank teller" was more likely than "bank teller," violating the laws of probability. (All feminist bank tellers are bank tellers, so the former can't be more likely). In this case System 1 substituted the easier question, "Is Linda a feminist?", neglecting the occupation qualifier. An alternative interpretation is that the subjects added an unstated cultural implicature to the effect that the other answer implied an exclusive or, that Linda was not a feminist.[2]
Optimism and loss aversion
Kahneman writes of a "pervasive optimistic bias", which "may well be the most significant of the cognitive biases." This bias generates the illusion of control: the illusion that we have substantial control of our lives.
A natural experiment reveals the prevalence of one kind of unwarranted optimism. The planning fallacy is the tendency to overestimate benefits and underestimate costs, impelling people to begin risky projects. In 2002, American kitchen remodeling was expected on average to cost $18,658, but actually cost $38,769.[2]
To explain overconfidence, Kahneman introduces the concept he terms What You See Is All There Is (WYSIATI). This theory states that when the mind makes decisions, it deals primarily with Known Knowns, phenomena it has observed already. It rarely considers Known Unknowns, phenomena that it knows to be relevant but about which it does not have information. Finally it appears oblivious to the possibility of Unknown Unknowns, unknown phenomena of unknown relevance.
He explains that humans fail to take into account complexity and that their understanding of the world consists of a small and necessarily un-representative set of observations. Furthermore, the mind generally does not account for the role of chance and therefore falsely assumes that a future event will be similar to a past event.
Framing is the context in which choices are presented. Experiment: subjects were asked whether they would opt for surgery if the "survival" rate is 90 percent, while others were told that the mortality rate is 10 percent. The first framing increased acceptance, even though the situation was no different.[11]
Rather than consider the odds that an incremental investment would produce a positive return, people tend to "throw good money after bad" and continue investing in projects with poor prospects that have already consumed significant resources. In part this is to avoid feelings of regret.[11]
This part (part III, sections 19–24) of the book is dedicated to the undue confidence in what the mind believes it knows. It suggests that people often overestimate how much they understand about the world and underestimate the role of chance in particular. This is related to the excessive certainty of hindsight, when an event seems to be understood after it has occurred or developed. Kahneman's opinions concerning overconfidence are influenced by Nassim Nicholas Taleb.[12]
Choices
In this section Kahneman returns to economics and expands his seminal work on Prospect Theory. He discusses the tendency for problems to be addressed in isolation and how, when other reference points are considered, the choice of that reference point (called a frame) has a disproportionate effect on the outcome. This section also offers advice on how some of the shortcomings of System 1 thinking can be avoided.
Kahneman developed prospect theory, the basis for his Nobel prize, to account for experimental errors he noticed in Daniel Bernoulli's traditional utility theory.[13] According to Kahneman, Utility Theory makes logical assumptions of economic rationality that do not represent people's actual choices, and does not take into account cognitive biases.
One example is that people are loss-averse: they are more likely to act to avert a loss than to achieve a gain. Another example is that the value people place on a change in probability (e.g., of winning something) depends on the reference point: people seem to place greater value on a change from 0% to 10% (going from impossibility to possibility) than from, say, 45% to 55%, and they place the greatest value of all on a change from 90% to 100% (going from possibility to certainty). This occurs despite the fact that by traditional utility theory all three changes give the same increase in utility. Consistent with loss-aversion, the order of the first and third of those is reversed when the event is presented as losing rather than winning something: there, the greatest value is placed on eliminating the probability of a loss to 0.
After the book's publication, the Journal of Economic Literature published a discussion of its parts concerning prospect theory,[14] as well as an analysis of the four fundamental factors on which it is based.[15]
Two selves
The fifth part of the book describes recent evidence which introduces a distinction between two selves, the 'experiencing self' and 'remembering self'.[16] Kahneman proposed an alternative measure that assessed pleasure or pain sampled from moment to moment, and then summed over time. Kahneman termed this "experienced" well-being and attached it to a separate "self." He distinguished this from the "remembered" well-being that the polls had attempted to measure. He found that these two measures of happiness diverged.[17]
Life as a story
The author's significant discovery was that the remembering self does not care about the duration of a pleasant or unpleasant experience. Instead, it retrospectively rates an experience by the maximum or minimum of the experience, and by the way it ends. The remembering self dominated the patient's ultimate conclusion.
"Odd as it may seem," Kahneman writes, "I am my remembering self, and the experiencing self, who does my living, is like a stranger to me."[3]
Experienced well-being
Kahneman first began the study of well-being in the 1990s. At the time most happiness research relied on polls about life satisfaction. Having previously studied unreliable memories, the author was doubtful that life satisfaction was a good indicator of happiness. He designed a question that emphasized instead the well-being of the experiencing self. The author proposed that "Helen was happy in the month of March" if she spent most of her time engaged in activities that she would rather continue than stop, little time in situations that she wished to escape, and not too much time in a neutral state that wouldn't prefer continuing or stopping the activity either way.
Kahneman suggests that emphasizing a life event such as a marriage or a new car can provide a distorted illusion of its true value. This "focusing illusion" revisits earlier ideas of substituting difficult questions and WYSIATI.
The book has achieved a large following among baseball scouts and baseball executives. The ways of thinking described in the book are believed to help scouts, who have to make major judgements off little information and can easily fall into prescriptive yet inaccurate patterns of analysis.[47]
The last chapter of Paul Bloom's Against Empathy discusses concepts also touched in Daniel Kahneman's book, Thinking, Fast and Slow, that suggest people make a series of rational and irrational decisions.[48][48]: 214 He criticizes the argument that "regardless of reason's virtues, we just aren't any good at it." His point is that people are not as "stupid as scholars think they are."[48]: 216 He explains that people are rational because they make thoughtful decisions in their everyday lives. For example, when someone has to make a big life decision they critically assess the outcomes, consequences, and alternative options.[48]: 230
Part of the book has been swept up in the replication crisis facing psychology and the social sciences. It was discovered many prominent research findings were difficult or impossible for others to replicate, and thus the original findings were called into question. An analysis[50] of the studies cited in chapter 4, "The Associative Machine", found that their replicability index (R-index)[51] is 14, indicating essentially low to no reliability. Kahneman himself responded to the study in blog comments and acknowledged the chapter's shortcomings: "I placed too much faith in underpowered studies."[52] Others have noted the irony in the fact that Kahneman made a mistake in judgment similar to the ones he studied.[53]
A later analysis[54] made a bolder claim that, despite Kahneman's previous contributions to the field of decision making, most of the book's ideas are based on 'scientific literature with shaky foundations'. A general lack of replication in the empirical studies cited in the book was given as a justification.
^Tversky, Amos; Kahneman, Daniel (September 1973). "Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability". Cognitive Psychology. 5 (2): 207–232. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9.(subscription required)
^Durr, Tony (February 1, 2014). "Thinking, Fast and Slow by Daniel Kahneman". American Journal of Education. 120 (2): 287–291. doi:10.1086/674372. ISSN0195-6744.
^Akst, Daniel (2011). "Rushing to Judgment". The Wilson Quarterly. 35 (4): 97–98. JSTOR41484407.
^Harrison, Kelly A. (2012). "Review of Thinking, Fast and Slow". Technical Communication. 59 (4): 342–343. JSTOR43093040.
^Richardson, Megan Lloyd (2012). "Review of Thinking, Fast and Slow [sic, included in a set of reviews]". The University of Toronto Law Journal. 62 (3): 453–457. doi:10.1353/tlj.2012.0013. JSTOR23263811. S2CID144044453.
^Vassallo, Philip (2012). "Review of Thinking, Fast and Slow". ETC: A Review of General Semantics. 69 (4): 480. JSTOR42579224.
Forza di difesa nazionale etiopeየኢፌዲሪ መከላከያ ሠራዊትEmblema della Forza di difesa nazionale etiope Descrizione generaleNazione Etiopia ServizioForze armate Dimensione150.000 attivi (2022)[1] Guarnigione/QGMinistero della difesa (Etiopia) Battaglie/guerreGuerra Adal-etiopeSpedizione britannica in AbissiniaGuerra egiziano-etiopeGuerra mahdistaGuerra d'AbissiniaGuerra d'EtiopiaSeconda guerra mondialeGuerra di CoreaGuerra d'indipendenza dell'EritreaGuerra di c...
Atentado de Berlim em 2016 Atentado de Berlim em 2016Percurso do caminhão na Breitscheidplatz. Local Berlim, Alemanha Data 19 de dezembro de 2016 20h00min (UTC +1) Tipo de ataque Atropelamento coletivo Alvo(s) Civis Arma(s) Caminhão semirreboque Scania R 450 Mortes 12 (11 pedestres e o dono do caminhão)[1] Feridos 56[1] Responsável(is) Estado Islâmico (reivindicou)[2] Suspeito(s) Anis Amri (morto pela polícia italiana em Milão)[3] Motivo Terrorismo islâmico Na noite de 19 de dezembro ...
Kreisgericht Ilmenau (1988) Das Kreisgericht war von 1952 bis 1990 in der DDR (und noch bis Ende 1992 in den neuen Bundesländern) ein Gericht der ordentlichen Gerichtsbarkeit erster Instanz. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Entstehung 2 Die Kreisgerichte 3 Rechtsgrundlagen 4 Aufhebung 5 Einzelnachweise 6 Literatur Entstehung Am 4. September 1945 wurde in der SBZ der SMAD-Befehl Nr. 49 über die Reorganisation des Gerichtswesens erlassen.[1] Es sollten diejenigen Gerichte wiederhergestellt werden...
Jalan Enggano adalah nama salah satu jalan utama Jakarta. Jalan ini menghubungkan Stasiun Tanjung Priok, Terminal Tanjung Priok dan Jalan Yos Sudarso/Jalan Raya Sulawesi. Nama jalan ini diambil dari nama salah satu pulau di Indonesia yaitu, Pulau Enggano. Jalan ini membentang sepanjang 1,1 km di Tanjung Priok, Tanjung Priok, Jakarta Utara. Jalan ini dilewati oleh bus Transjakarta koridor 10. Jalan ini cukup padat, akibat banyak truk kontainer yang melewati jalan ini dari dan menuju Pelab...
Hồng y Egano Righi-Lambertini Thành viên phân bộ Ngoại thường Giáo triều Rôma (1979 - 1990) Giáo hộiCông giáo RômaTruyền chứcThụ phongNgày 25 tháng 5 năm 1929Tấn phongNgày 28 tháng 10 năm 1960Thăng Hồng yNgày 30 tháng 6 năm 1979Thông tin cá nhânSinhNgày 22 tháng 2 năm 1906MấtNgày 4 tháng 10 năm 2000 Cách xưng hô vớiEgano Righi-LambertiniDanh hiệuĐức Hồng YTrang trọngĐức Hồng YSau khi qua đờiĐức Cố Hồng YTh...
Die Apotheke am Molkenmarkt (meist benannt nach den jeweiligen Besitzern, auch Zorn'sche Apotheke) war eine der ältesten Apotheken Berlins. Sie bestand von 1556 bis 1834. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Lage 2 Geschichte 3 Persönlichkeiten 4 Literatur 5 Einzelnachweise Lage Molkenmarkt und Mühlendamm, 1688, Nr. 4 an der oberen rechten Ecke des dreieckigen Platzes (ob das Haus tatsächlich so aussah, ist unsicher) Die Apotheke lag zuerst auf der südlichen Seite des Molkenmarktes an der Stralauer Stra...
Indian cinematographer This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages) This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification. Please help by adding reliable sources. Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page, especially if potentially libelous.Find sources: V. Ma...
Este artículo o sección necesita referencias que aparezcan en una publicación acreditada.Este aviso fue puesto el 16 de marzo de 2021. La versión actual de este artículo o sección parece estar escrita a modo de publicidad.Para satisfacer los estándares de calidad de Wikipedia y procurar un punto de vista neutral, este artículo o sección puede requerir limpieza. Por favor discute este problema en la página de discusión. Este aviso fue puesto el 16 de marzo de 2021. Alianza Estratég...
French nobleman, revolutionary, and politician Pierre Napoleon BonaparteBorn(1815-10-11)11 October 1815Rome, ItalyDied7 April 1881(1881-04-07) (aged 65)Versailles, FranceBurialCimetière des Gonards, Versailles, FranceSpouseÉléonore-Justine RuflinIssueRoland Napoleon BonaparteJeanne, Marquise de Villeneuve-EscaplonHouseBonaparteFatherLucien BonaparteMotherAlexandrine de Bleschamp Prince Pierre-Napoléon Bonaparte (11 October 1815 – 7 April 1881) was a French nobleman, revolutionary a...
Removal of Russian influence in Ukraine Not to be confused with Ukrainization or Decommunization in Ukraine.You can help expand this article with text translated from the corresponding article in Ukrainian. (November 2023) Click [show] for important translation instructions. View a machine-translated version of the Ukrainian article. Machine translation, like DeepL or Google Translate, is a useful starting point for translations, but translators must revise errors as necessary and confir...
American geologist Julia Ann (Jan) TullisBornJulia Ann TullisNationalityAmericanAlma materCarleton College (A.B.) University of California, Los Angeles (PhD)Known forDeformation MechanicsScientific careerFieldsGeologyRock mechanicsRheologyInstitutionsUniversity of California, Los AngelesBrown UniversityThesisPreferred orientations in experimentally deformed quartzites (1971)Doctoral advisorDavid T. Griggs, John ChristieDoctoral studentsGreg Hirth Julia Ann “Jan” Tullis is a...
1939 film by Gregory Ratoff Wife, Husband and FriendOpening titlesDirected byGregory RatoffWritten byNunnally JohnsonBased onTwo Can Singby James M. CainProduced byDarryl F. ZanuckStarringLoretta YoungWarner BaxterBinnie BarnesCesar RomeroGeorge BarbierCinematographyErnest PalmerMusic byAlfred NewmanProductioncompanyTwentieth Century Fox Film Corp.Release date February 25, 1939 (1939-02-25) Running time75 minutesCountryUnited StatesLanguageEnglish Wife, Husband and Friend is a ...
Warner Bros. theatrical cartoon character This article's lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points. Please consider expanding the lead to provide an accessible overview of all important aspects of the article. (July 2023) Fictional character Marvin the MartianLooney Tunes/Merrie Melodies characterFirst appearanceHaredevil Hare (July 24, 1948; 75 years ago (1948-07-24))Created byChuck JonesVoiced by Mel Blanc (1948–1986) Joe Alaskey (1991–2008) ...
Perepat Perepat, Sonneratia alba dari Sangatta, Kalimantan Timur Klasifikasi ilmiah Kerajaan: Plantae Divisi: Magnoliophyta Kelas: Magnoliopsida Ordo: Myrtales Famili: Lythraceae Genus: Sonneratia Spesies: S. alba Nama binomial Sonneratia alba J.E. Smith Sinonim Chiratia leucantha Montr., Mangium caseolare album Rumph., Rhizophora caseolaris Linné, Sonneratia acida Benth., Sonneratia alba F. Vill., Sonneratia caseolaris Engl., Sonneratia griffithii (non Kurz) Watson, Sonneratia iriomote...
Nuclear research project This article is an orphan, as no other articles link to it. Please introduce links to this page from related articles; try the Find link tool for suggestions. (March 2021) Figure 1: Outside view of the nuclear power plant in Brokdorf CONUS (COherent Neutrino nUcleus Scattering) Experiment is a research project at the commercial nuclear power plant in Brokdorf, Germany (see Figure 1). The CONUS project is sponsored by the Max-Planck-Institut für Kernphysik and Preusse...
Mestaruussarja 1952 Competizione Mestaruussarja Sport Calcio Edizione 43ª Organizzatore SPL/FBF Luogo Finlandia Partecipanti 10 Formula Girone all'italiana Risultati Vincitore KTP(2º titolo) Secondo VIFK Retrocessioni TPKTPS Statistiche Miglior marcatore Mauri Vanhanen (16) Incontri disputati 90 Gol segnati 322 (3,58 per incontro) Cronologia della competizione 1951 1953 Manuale La Mestaruussarja 1952 fu la quarantatreesima edizione della massima serie del campionato finlande...
One gene variant masking the effect of another in the other copy of the gene This article needs additional citations for verification. Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.Find sources: Dominance genetics – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (February 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) Autosomal dominant and autosomal recessive in...
One of the club's premises, 106 Piccadilly, in 2006 The St James's Club was a London gentlemen's club which operated between 1857 and 1978. It was founded by two leading diplomats and its members continued to be largely diplomats and authors. It was first established in Charles Street and moved to 106 Piccadilly by 1868. In the final quarter of the twentieth century many gentlemen’s clubs of London suffered from declining membership, and in 1978 the St James's Club merged with Brooks's Club...